Questões de Concurso Militar ITA 2009 para Aluno - Português e Inglês
Foram encontradas 40 questões
A questão refere-se ao índice da Revista TIME.
A questão refere-se ao índice da Revista TIME.
A questão refere-se ao índice da Revista TIME.
De acordo com o texto:
I. O crescimento do índice de vegetarianos tornou os moradores de Palermo mais saudáveis.
II. O modo de vida dos vegetarianos é bem visto pelos argentinos.
III. A adesão a hábitos vegetarianos é consequência do custo da carne vermelha.
IV. Bio Restaurante e La Esquina de las Flores são os principais restaurantes vegetarianos da Argentina.
Está(ão) correta(s)
De acordo com o texto:
I. Palermo pertence a uma região rica, cercada por fazendas de gado.
II. A cidade de Palermo sedia um evento anual de agropecuária no mês de julho.
III. Há muitos vegetarianos em Palermo.
Está(ão) correta(s)
Assinale a opção em que o termo da coluna II NÃO pode substituir o termo da coluna I no texto.
Leia o seguinte período extraído do texto:
Vegetarian restaurants have lower overheads since they don’t need freezers, says Marisa Ledesma, one of the owners of Bio Restaurante, a smart eatery. (parágrafo 2).
Assinale a opção que pode substituir o termo since sem que o sentido da oração seja comprometido.
Considere a tradução dos seguintes trechos extraídos do texto:
I. ...though Mr Fujii got 15 years. (parágrafo 1)
...embora Mr Fujii tenha recebido (uma pena de) 15 anos.
II. ...because eligible jurors (…) were away fighting. (parágrafo 2)
…porque os jurados elegíveis (…) estavam na guerra.
III. ...With Japan about to hold an election ... (parágrafo 4)
Com o Japão prestes a realizar uma eleição ...
Está(ão) correta(s)
Considere as seguintes afirmações:
I. O texto apresenta o caso ocorrido com um cidadão japonês acusado de atacar seu vizinho.
II. De acordo com as regras vigentes, o corpo de jurados japonês é formado por profissionais da área jurídica e por cidadãos comuns.
III. Mr. Maruta é o grande responsável pela reformulação do sistema penal no Japão e em outros países asiáticos.
Está(ão) correta(s)
De acordo com o texto:
I. Países como a Coréia do Sul e Taiwan têm os mesmos índices de criminalidade que o Japão.
II. É crescente o número de japoneses interessados em atuar como jurados no Japão.
III. Cidadãos comuns que participam de julgamentos como jurados podem sentenciar a pena capital.
Está(ão) correta(s)
Com relação ao caso de Katsuyoshi Fujii, pode-se afirmar que
I. o júri condenou-o a 15 anos de prisão.
II. houve acareação entre o acusado, Sr. Fujii, e o filho da vítima.
III. por ser um caso comum de julgamento, mais de duas mil pessoas se candidataram a participar do corpo de jurados.
Está(ão) correta(s)
In August of 2000, a Japanese scientist named Toshiyuki Nakagaki announced that he had trained an amoebalike organism called slime mold to find the shortest route through a maze. Nakagaki had placed the mold in a small maze comprising four possible routes and planted pieces of food at two of the exits. Despite its being an incredibly primitive organism (a close relative of ordinary fungi) with no centralized brain whatsoever, the slime mold managed to plot the most efficient route to the food, stretching its body through the maze so that it connected directly to the two food sources. Without any apparent cognitive resources, the slime mold had “solved” the maze puzzle.
For such a simple organism, the slime mold has an impressive intellectual pedigree. Nakagaki’s announcement was only the latest in a long chain of investigations into the subtleties of slime mold behavior. For scientists trying to understand systems that use relatively simple components to build higher-level intelligence, the slime mold may someday be seen as the equivalent of the finches and tortoises that Darwin observed on the Galapagos Islands.
How did such a lowly organism come to play such an important scientific role? That story begins in the late sixties in New York City, with a scientist named Evelyn Fox Keller. A Harvard Ph.D. in physics, Keller had written her dissertation on molecular biology, and she had spent some time exploring the nascent field of “non-equilibrium thermodynamics”, which in later years would come to be associated with complexity theory. By 1968, she was working as an associate at Sloan-Kettering in Manhattan, thinking about the application of mathematics to biological problems. Mathematics had played such a tremendous role in expanding our understanding of physics, Keller thought – so perhaps it might also be useful for understanding living systems.
In the spring of 1968, Keller met a visiting scholar named Lee Segel, an applied mathematician who shared her interests. It was Segel who first introduced her to the bizarre conduct of the slime mold, and together they began a series of investigations that would help transform not just our understanding of biological development but also the disparate worlds of brain science, software design, and urban studies.
(…)
Johson, Steven. Emergence. Peguin Books Ltd. 2001, pp. 11-12.
In August of 2000, a Japanese scientist named Toshiyuki Nakagaki announced that he had trained an amoebalike organism called slime mold to find the shortest route through a maze. Nakagaki had placed the mold in a small maze comprising four possible routes and planted pieces of food at two of the exits. Despite its being an incredibly primitive organism (a close relative of ordinary fungi) with no centralized brain whatsoever, the slime mold managed to plot the most efficient route to the food, stretching its body through the maze so that it connected directly to the two food sources. Without any apparent cognitive resources, the slime mold had “solved” the maze puzzle.
For such a simple organism, the slime mold has an impressive intellectual pedigree. Nakagaki’s announcement was only the latest in a long chain of investigations into the subtleties of slime mold behavior. For scientists trying to understand systems that use relatively simple components to build higher-level intelligence, the slime mold may someday be seen as the equivalent of the finches and tortoises that Darwin observed on the Galapagos Islands.
How did such a lowly organism come to play such an important scientific role? That story begins in the late sixties in New York City, with a scientist named Evelyn Fox Keller. A Harvard Ph.D. in physics, Keller had written her dissertation on molecular biology, and she had spent some time exploring the nascent field of “non-equilibrium thermodynamics”, which in later years would come to be associated with complexity theory. By 1968, she was working as an associate at Sloan-Kettering in Manhattan, thinking about the application of mathematics to biological problems. Mathematics had played such a tremendous role in expanding our understanding of physics, Keller thought – so perhaps it might also be useful for understanding living systems.
In the spring of 1968, Keller met a visiting scholar named Lee Segel, an applied mathematician who shared her interests. It was Segel who first introduced her to the bizarre conduct of the slime mold, and together they began a series of investigations that would help transform not just our understanding of biological development but also the disparate worlds of brain science, software design, and urban studies.
(…)
Johson, Steven. Emergence. Peguin Books Ltd. 2001, pp. 11-12.
Em sua pesquisa, Toshiyuki Nakagaki
I. colocou um slime mold num labirinto com quatro saídas.
II. treinou um slime mold a sair de um labirinto pelo caminho mais curto.
III. colocou alimentos em todas as saídas do labirinto para atrair o slime mold.
Está(ão) correta(s)
In August of 2000, a Japanese scientist named Toshiyuki Nakagaki announced that he had trained an amoebalike organism called slime mold to find the shortest route through a maze. Nakagaki had placed the mold in a small maze comprising four possible routes and planted pieces of food at two of the exits. Despite its being an incredibly primitive organism (a close relative of ordinary fungi) with no centralized brain whatsoever, the slime mold managed to plot the most efficient route to the food, stretching its body through the maze so that it connected directly to the two food sources. Without any apparent cognitive resources, the slime mold had “solved” the maze puzzle.
For such a simple organism, the slime mold has an impressive intellectual pedigree. Nakagaki’s announcement was only the latest in a long chain of investigations into the subtleties of slime mold behavior. For scientists trying to understand systems that use relatively simple components to build higher-level intelligence, the slime mold may someday be seen as the equivalent of the finches and tortoises that Darwin observed on the Galapagos Islands.
How did such a lowly organism come to play such an important scientific role? That story begins in the late sixties in New York City, with a scientist named Evelyn Fox Keller. A Harvard Ph.D. in physics, Keller had written her dissertation on molecular biology, and she had spent some time exploring the nascent field of “non-equilibrium thermodynamics”, which in later years would come to be associated with complexity theory. By 1968, she was working as an associate at Sloan-Kettering in Manhattan, thinking about the application of mathematics to biological problems. Mathematics had played such a tremendous role in expanding our understanding of physics, Keller thought – so perhaps it might also be useful for understanding living systems.
In the spring of 1968, Keller met a visiting scholar named Lee Segel, an applied mathematician who shared her interests. It was Segel who first introduced her to the bizarre conduct of the slime mold, and together they began a series of investigations that would help transform not just our understanding of biological development but also the disparate worlds of brain science, software design, and urban studies.
(…)
Johson, Steven. Emergence. Peguin Books Ltd. 2001, pp. 11-12.
De acordo com o texto, Evelyn Fox Keller
I. tornou-se PhD em Física pela Universidade de Harvard e foi a pioneira nos estudos sobre teoria de sistemas complexos.
II. acreditava na importância da Matemática não apenas para o estudo da Física, mas também da Biologia.
III. Influenciou as pesquisas do matemático Lee Segel, levando-o a se interessar pelo comportamento dos slime molds.
Está(ão) correta(s)
In August of 2000, a Japanese scientist named Toshiyuki Nakagaki announced that he had trained an amoebalike organism called slime mold to find the shortest route through a maze. Nakagaki had placed the mold in a small maze comprising four possible routes and planted pieces of food at two of the exits. Despite its being an incredibly primitive organism (a close relative of ordinary fungi) with no centralized brain whatsoever, the slime mold managed to plot the most efficient route to the food, stretching its body through the maze so that it connected directly to the two food sources. Without any apparent cognitive resources, the slime mold had “solved” the maze puzzle.
For such a simple organism, the slime mold has an impressive intellectual pedigree. Nakagaki’s announcement was only the latest in a long chain of investigations into the subtleties of slime mold behavior. For scientists trying to understand systems that use relatively simple components to build higher-level intelligence, the slime mold may someday be seen as the equivalent of the finches and tortoises that Darwin observed on the Galapagos Islands.
How did such a lowly organism come to play such an important scientific role? That story begins in the late sixties in New York City, with a scientist named Evelyn Fox Keller. A Harvard Ph.D. in physics, Keller had written her dissertation on molecular biology, and she had spent some time exploring the nascent field of “non-equilibrium thermodynamics”, which in later years would come to be associated with complexity theory. By 1968, she was working as an associate at Sloan-Kettering in Manhattan, thinking about the application of mathematics to biological problems. Mathematics had played such a tremendous role in expanding our understanding of physics, Keller thought – so perhaps it might also be useful for understanding living systems.
In the spring of 1968, Keller met a visiting scholar named Lee Segel, an applied mathematician who shared her interests. It was Segel who first introduced her to the bizarre conduct of the slime mold, and together they began a series of investigations that would help transform not just our understanding of biological development but also the disparate worlds of brain science, software design, and urban studies.
(…)
Johson, Steven. Emergence. Peguin Books Ltd. 2001, pp. 11-12.
In August of 2000, a Japanese scientist named Toshiyuki Nakagaki announced that he had trained an amoebalike organism called slime mold to find the shortest route through a maze. Nakagaki had placed the mold in a small maze comprising four possible routes and planted pieces of food at two of the exits. Despite its being an incredibly primitive organism (a close relative of ordinary fungi) with no centralized brain whatsoever, the slime mold managed to plot the most efficient route to the food, stretching its body through the maze so that it connected directly to the two food sources. Without any apparent cognitive resources, the slime mold had “solved” the maze puzzle.
For such a simple organism, the slime mold has an impressive intellectual pedigree. Nakagaki’s announcement was only the latest in a long chain of investigations into the subtleties of slime mold behavior. For scientists trying to understand systems that use relatively simple components to build higher-level intelligence, the slime mold may someday be seen as the equivalent of the finches and tortoises that Darwin observed on the Galapagos Islands.
How did such a lowly organism come to play such an important scientific role? That story begins in the late sixties in New York City, with a scientist named Evelyn Fox Keller. A Harvard Ph.D. in physics, Keller had written her dissertation on molecular biology, and she had spent some time exploring the nascent field of “non-equilibrium thermodynamics”, which in later years would come to be associated with complexity theory. By 1968, she was working as an associate at Sloan-Kettering in Manhattan, thinking about the application of mathematics to biological problems. Mathematics had played such a tremendous role in expanding our understanding of physics, Keller thought – so perhaps it might also be useful for understanding living systems.
In the spring of 1968, Keller met a visiting scholar named Lee Segel, an applied mathematician who shared her interests. It was Segel who first introduced her to the bizarre conduct of the slime mold, and together they began a series of investigations that would help transform not just our understanding of biological development but also the disparate worlds of brain science, software design, and urban studies.
(…)
Johson, Steven. Emergence. Peguin Books Ltd. 2001, pp. 11-12.
Considere as seguintes afirmações:
I. As listas verticais indicadas afinam a silhueta.
II. A figura mostra sapatos que não se desgastam com o tempo.
III. Inactive Wear é apropriada para praticantes de exercícios físicos.
Está(ão) correta(s):